Análise do campo conceitual da engenharia de sistemas cognitivos e proposta de uma nova agenda de pesquisa
Analysis of the conceptual field of cognitive systems engineering and the proposal of a new research agenda
Henriqson, Éder; Saurin, Tarcisio Abreu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-65132013005000056
Production, vol.24, n2, p.405-419, 2014
Resumo
Este artigo tem por objetivo descrever a evolução do campo conceitual da Engenharia de Sistemas Cognitivos (ESC), discutir seus fundamentos epistemológicos e verificar a sua coerência com a natureza dos problemas investigados. Para tanto, o trabalho apresenta uma revisão bibliográfica do desenvolvimento histórico desse campo e a caracterização de cinco abordagens comuns nas pesquisas em ESC. Os resultados apontam que os estudos em ESC, via de regra, tomam o indivíduo ou o sistema técnico como unidade de análise, deixando lacunas importantes na investigação de interações entre humano, trabalho e artefatos. Uma nova agenda de pesquisa é proposta tendo como base três premissas: um sistema cognitivo não deve ser interpretado como uma máquina cartesiano-newtoniana, que possui relações evidentes de causa e efeito; um sistema cognitivo não pode ser modelado com precisão e perfeição; a investigação de sistemas cognitivos deve focar em fenômenos emergentes das interações entre humano, trabalho e artefatos tecnológicos.
Palavras-chave
Engenharia de sistemas cognitivos. Sistemas sociotécnicos complexos. Possibilidades de agenda de pesquisa
Abstract
This article aims to describe the evolution of the conceptual field of cognitive systems engineering (CSE), to discuss its epistemological foundations and to verify the coherence of CSE with the nature of the problems that are investigated. Toward this aim, this work presents a literature review of the historical development of CSE and characterizes five common approaches in CSE research. The results indicate that studies in CSE have, as a rule, assumed the individual or the technical system to be the unit of analysis, ignoring important opportunities in the investigation of the interactions among human, work, and artifacts. A new research agenda is proposed based on an analysis of three assumptions: a cognitive system should not be assumed to be a Cartesian-Newtonian machine, which has clear cause-and-effect relationships; a cognitive system cannot be modeled with precision and perfection; and the investigation of cognitive systems should focus on emergent phenomena related to the interactions among human, work, and technological artifacts.
Keywords
gnitive systems engineering. Complex socio-technical systems. Possibilities for a research agenda.
References
ALBRECHTSEN, H. et al. Affordance in activity theory and cognitive systems engineering (RISO-r-1287(EN)). Roskield: RISO National Laboratory, 2001.
ASHBY, W. R. Requisite variety and its implications for the control of complex systems. Cybernetica, v. 1, p. 83-99, 1959.
BAKER, D.; DISMUKES, R. A Framework for Understanding Crew Performance Assessment Issues. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, v. 12, n. 3, p. 205-222, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327108IJAP1203_2
BAARS, B. J. The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press, 1986.
BEM, S.; KEIJZER, F. Recent changes in the concept of cognition. Theory and Psychology, v. 6, p. 449-469, 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354396063006
BISANTZ, A. M.; VICENTE, K. J. Making the abstraction hierarchy concrete. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, v. 40, n. 1, p. 83-117, 1994. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ijhc.1994.1005
BOVAIR, S.; KIERAS, D. E.; POLSON, P. G. The acquisition and performance of text editing skill: A cognitive complexity analysis. Human-Computer Interaction, v. 5, p. 1-48, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci0501_1
BURNS, K. Improving Intuition with Bayesian Boxes: On Cognitive Difficulties in Combining Probabilities. In: HOFFMAN, R. (Ed.). Expertise out of the context. Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Naturalistic Decision Making. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007. p. 263-280.
CARD, S. K.; MORAN, T. P.; NEWELL, A. The keystroke-level model for user performance time with interactive systems. Communications of the ACM, v. 23, n. 7, p. 396-410, 1980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/358886.358895
CARD, S. K.; MORAN, T. P.; NEWELL, A. The psychology of computer-human interaction. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 1983.
CARD, S. K.; MORAN, T. P.; NEWELL, A. The model human processor: An engineering model of human performance. In: BOFF, K.; KAUFMAN, L.; THOMAS, J. (Eds.). Handbook of perception and human performance, Vol II. Cognitive Processes and Performance. New York: Wiley, 1986.
CILLIERS, P. Boundaries, Hierarchies and Networks in Complex Systems. International Journal of Innovation Management, v. 5, n. 2, p. 135-147, 2001.
CILLIERS, P. Complexity, Deconstruction and Relativism. Theory Culture & Society, v. 22, n. 5, p. 255-267, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0263276405058052
CHRISTOFFERSEN, K.; WOODS, D. D. Making sense of change: Extracting events from dynamic process data. Columbus: The Ohio State University, 2003. Institute for Ergonomics/Cognitive Systems Engineering Laboratory Report ERGO-CSEL 01-TR-02.
COOK, R. I. Adapting to the new technology in the operating room. Human Factors, v. 38, n. 4, p. 593-613, 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872096778827224
COOK, R. I.; WOODS, D. D.; HOWIE, M. B. Unintentional delivery of vasoactive drugs with an electromechanical infusion device. Journal of Cardiothoracic and Vascular Anesthesia, v. 6, p. 1-7, 1992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1053-0770(92)90207-N
CRANDALL, B.; KLEIN, G.; HOFFMAN, R. Working Minds: a practitioner's guide to cognitive task analysis. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2006.
DEKKER, S. W. A. Crew situation awareness in high-tech settings: Tactics for research into an ill-defined phenomenon. Transportation Human Factors, v. 2, p. 49-62, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/STHF0201_7
DEKKER, S. W. A. Ten questions about human error. Aldershot: Ashgate, 2005a.
DEKKER, S. W. A. Why we need new accident models? Lund: Lund University School of Aviation, 2005b. Technical Report 2005-02.
DEKKER, S. W. A. Complexity, signal detection, and the application of ergonomics: reflections on a healthcare study. Journal of Applied Ergonomics, v. 43, p. 468-472, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2011.07.003
DEKKER, S. W. A. et al. Complicated, Complex and Compliant: best practices in obstetrics. Cognition, Technology, and Work. 2012. In press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10111-011-0211-6
DEKKER, S. W. A.; CELLIERS, F. P.; HOFMEYR, J. H. The complexity of failure: implications of complexity theory for safety investigations. Safety Science, v. 49, n. 6, p. 939-945, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2011.01.008
DEKKER, S. W. A.; HOLLNAGEL, E. Human factors and folk models. Cognition, Technology, and Work, v. 6, p. 79-86, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10111-003-0136-9
DEKKER, S. W. A. et al. Epistemological self-confidence in human factors research. Journal of Cognition and Decision Making, v. 4, n. 1, p. 27-38, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/155534310X495573
DEKKER, S. W. A.; WOODS, D. D. MABA-MABA or abracadabra? Progress on human automation coordination. Cognition, Technology, and Work, v. 4, p. 40-244, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s101110200022
EGGLESTON, R. G. Cognitive systems engineering at 20-something: where do we stand? In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 13-77.
ENDSLEY, M. R. Toward a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors, v. 37, p. 32-64, 1995a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872095779049543
ENDSLEY, M. R. Measurement of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors, v. 37, p. 65-84, 1995b. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872095779049499
ENDSLEY, M. R.; KABER, D. B. Level of automation effects on performance, situation awareness and workload in a dynamic control task. Ergonomics, v. 42, p. 462-492, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/001401399185595
ENDSLEY, M. R.; KIRIS, E. O. The out-of-the-loop performance problem and level of control in automation. Human Factors, v. 37, p. 381-394, 1995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872095779064555
ENDSLEY, M. R.; GARLAND, D. J. (Eds.). Situation Awareness Analysis and Measurement. Mahawah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2000.
ENDSLEY, M. R. et al. Cognitive engineering and decision making: an overview and future course. Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, v. 1, n. 1, p. 1-21, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/155534340700100101
FEDERAL AVIATION ADMINISTRATION - FAA. Aeronautical Decision Making. Advisoty Circular 60-22. Washington: Federal Aviation Administration, 1991.
FLACH, J. M. The ecology of human-machine systems I: Introduction. Ecological Psychology, v. 2, n. 3, p. 191-205, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0203_1
FLIN, R.; O'CONNOR, P.; CRICHTON, M. Safety at the sharp end: a guide to non-technical skills. Burlington: Ashgate, 2008.
GAUTHEREU, V.; HOLLNAGEL, E. Planning, control and adaptation. European Management journal, v. 23, n. 1, p. 118-131, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.emj.2004.12.016
GIBBSON, J. J. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1979.
GRAY, W. D.; JOHN, B. E.; ATWOOD, M. E. The Précis of Project Ernestine or an overview of a validation of GOMS. In: BAUERSFELD, P.; BENNETT, J.; LYNCH, G. (Eds.). Proceedings of the ACM CHI'92 Conference on Human factors in Computing Systems. New York: ACM Press, 1992. p. 307-3012.
HART, S. G. Theoretical basis for workload assessment research at NASA - Armes Research Center. Defense Technical Information Center, 1982. p. 445-470. Proceedings of the workshop on flight testing to identify pilot workload and pilot dynamics (AFTECTR-82-5).
HARRÉ, R. The Rediscovery of the human mind: The discursive approach. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, v. 2, p. 43-62, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.00025
HATFIELD, G. Psychology, philosophy, and cognitive science: Reflections on the history and philosophy of experimental psychology. Mind & Language, v. 17, p. 207-232, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00196
HENDY, K. C. et al. Analysing cognitive system from a perceptual control point of view. In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 201-250.
HICK, W. E. On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, v. 4, p. 11-26, 1952. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470215208416600
HYMAN, R. Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, v. 45, p. 188-196, 1953. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0056940
HOFFMAN, R. et al. A rose by any other name...would probably be given an acronym. IEEE Inteligent Systems, v. 17, n. 4, p. 72-80, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/MIS.2002.1024755
HOFFMAN, R.; WOODS, D. D. Special Section on Studying Cognitive Systems in Context. Human Factors, v. 42, n. 1, p. 1-7, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872000779656633
HOLLNAGEL, E. Context, cognition, and control. In: WAERN, Y. (Ed.). Co-operative in process management: Cognition and information technology. London: Taylor & Francis, 1998. p. 27-51.
HOLLNAGEL, E. Modeling the controller of a process. Transactions of the Institute of Measurement and Control, v. 21, n. 4-5, p. 163-170, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014233129902100404
HOLLNAGEL, E. Time and Time Again. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, v. 3, n. 2, p. 143-158, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14639220210124111
HOLLNAGEL, E. Prolegomenon to Cognitive Task Design. In: HOLLNAGEL, E. (Ed.). Handbook of Cognitive Task Design. Mahawah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2003. p. 03-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781410607775
HOLLNAGEL, E. Barriers and accident prevention. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing, 2004.
HOLLNAGEL, E. Human reliability assessment in context. Nuclear Engineering and Technology, v. 37, n. 2, p. 159-166, 2005.
HOLLNAGEL, E. The ETTO Principle Efficiency-Thoroughness Trade-Off. Surrey: Ashgate Publishing Company, 2009.
HOLLNAGEL, E.; WOODS, D. D. Cognitive systems engineering: New wine in new bottles. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, v. 18, p. 583-600, 1983. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0020-7373(83)80034-0
HOLLNAGEL, E.; WOODS, D. D. Joint Cognitive Systems: foundations of cognitive systems engineering. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781420038194
HUTCHINS, E. How a cockpit remembers its speeds. Cognitive Science, v. 19, p. 265-288, 1995a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1903_1
HUTCHINS, E. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1995b.
JOHANSSON, B.; HOLLNAGEL, E. Pre-requisites for large scale coordination. Cognition, Technology, and Work, v. 9, p. 5-13, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10111-006-0050-z
JOHNSON-LAIRD, P. N. Mental models in cognitive science. Cognitive Science, v. 4, p. 7-115, 1980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0401_4
KABER, D. B.; ENDSLEY, M. R. The effects of level of automation and adaptive automation on human performance, situation awareness and workload in a dynamic control task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, v. 5, n. 2, p. 113-153, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1463922021000054335
KABER, D. B.; RILEY, J. Adaptive automation of a dynamic control task based on secondary task workload measurement. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, v. 3, n. 3, p. 169-187, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327566ijce0303_1
KIERAS, D. E. Toward a practical GOMS model methodology for user interface design. In: HELANDER, M. (Ed.). The handbook of human-computer interaction. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1988. p. 135-158.
KIERAS, D. E. A guide to GOMS model usability evaluation using NGOMSL. 1996. Disponível em:
KIERAS, D. E.; BOVAIR, S. The role of a mental model in learning to operate a device. Cognitive Science, v. 8, p. 255-273, 1984. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0803_3
KIERAS, D. E.; POLSON, P. G. An approach to the formal analysis of user complexity. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, v. 22, p. 365-394, 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0020-7373(85)80045-6
LASZLO, J. Narrative psychology's contribution to second cognitive revolution. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, v. 2, p. 337-354, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/JCEP.2.2004.3-4.10
LETSKY, M. P. et al. (Eds.). Macrocognition in teams: theories and methodologies. Aldershot: Ashgate, 2008.
LOPES, E. J.; LOPES, R.; TEIXEIRA, R. F. A psicologia cognitiva experimental cinquenta anos depois: a crise do paradigma do processamento de informação. Paidea, v. 14, n. 27, p. 17-26, 2004.
LOUKOPOULOS, L. D.; DISMUKES, R. K.; BARSHI, I. The multitasking myth: Handling complexity in real-world operations. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing Co, 2009.
McCLELLAND, J. L. On the time relations of mental processes. An examination of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, v. 86, p. 287-330, 1979. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.86.4.287
McCULLOCH, W.; PITTS, W. A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, v. 5, p. 15-133, 1943. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02478259
McNEESE, M. D. Discovering how cognitive systems engineering should be engineered for aviation domains: a developmental look at work, research, and practice. In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 79-119.
MEYER, D. E.; KIERAS, D. E. A computational theory of executive control processes and human multiple task performance: Part 1: Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, v. 104, p. 3-65, 1995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.3
MILLER, J. Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: Theoretical distinctions and empirical results. Acta Psychologica, v. 67, p. 191-257, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(88)90013-3
MILLER, G. A. The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, v. 7, p. 141-144, 2003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9
NAIKAR, N.; SANDERSON, P. M. Work domain analysis for training-system definition and acquisition. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, v. 9, n. 3, p. 271-290, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327108ijap0903_5
NAIKAR, N.; SANDERSON, P. M.; LINTERN, G. F/A-18 work domain analysis: identification of training needs for simulator acquisition. Melbourne: Aeronautical and Maritime Research Laboratory, 2000. DSTO Client Report, DSTO-CR-0117.
NEISSER, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.
NEISSER, U. Cognition and Reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1976.
NEMETH, C.; HOLLNAGEL, E.; DEKKER, S. W. A. (Eds.). Resilience engineering perspectives, Volume 2: preparation and restoration. Burlington: Ashgate, 2009.
NEWELL, A. Physical symbol systems. Cognitive science, v. 4, p. 135-183, 1980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0402_2
NEWELL, A.; SHAW, J. C.; SIMON, H. A. Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review, v. 65, p. 151-166, 1958. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0048495
NORMAN, D. A. Steps towards a cognitive engineering. San Diego: University of California, Program in Cognitive Science, 1981. Technical Report.
NORMAN, D. A. Cognitive engineering. In: NORMAN, D. A.; DRAPER, S. W. (Eds.). Usercentered system design. Mahwah: Erlbaum, 1986. p. 31-61.
O'DONOHUE, W.; FERGUSON, K. E.; NAUGLE, A. E. The structure of the cognitive revolution: an examination from the philosophy of science. The Behavior Analyst, v. 26, p. 85-110, 2003.
OLSON, J. R.; OLSON, G. M. The growth of cognitive modeling in human-computer interaction since GOMS. Human-Computer Interaction, v. 5, p. 221-265, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci0502&3_4
PARASURAMAN, R.; RILEY, V. Humans and automation: Use, misuse, disuse and abuse. Human Factors, n. 39, p. 230-253, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872097778543886
PARASURAMAN, R.; SHERIDAN, T. B.; WICKENS, C. D. A model of types and levels of human interaction with automation. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, n. 30, p. 286-297, 2000.
PARASURAMAN, R.; SHERIDAN, T. B.; WICKENS, C. D. Situation awareness, mental workload and trust in automation: Viable empirically supported cognitive engineering constructs. Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making. Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, v. 2, p. 140-160, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/155534308X284417
PIAGET, J. Estudos Sociológicos. Rio de Janeiro: Forense, 1973.
POTTER, S. S. et al. Bridging the gap between cognitive analysis and effective decision aiding. In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 137-168.
POSNER, M. I.; DEHAENE, S. Attentional networks. Trends in neurosciences, v. 17, p. 75-79, 1994. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7
POSNER, M. I. et al. Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, v. 240, p. 1627-1631, 1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3289116
RASMUSSEN, J. Trends in human reliability analysis. Ergonomics, v. 28, n. 8, p. 1185-1195, 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140138508963241
RASMUSSEN, J. Information processing and human-machine interaction; an approach to cognitive engineering. New York: Elsevier Science Publishing, 1986.
RASMUSSEN, J.; PEJTERSEN, A. M.; GOODSTEIN, L. P. Cognitive systems engineering. New York: Wiley, 1994.
RASMUSSEN, J.; PEJTERNSEN, A. M.; SHMIDT, K. Taxonomy for Cognitive Work Analysis. Roskield: RISO National Laboratory, 1990.
REASON, J. Human error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139062367
RESING, J. M. Cognitive engineering and its relation to future aviation systems. In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 1-12.
SARTER, N. B.; AMALBERTI, R. (Eds.). Cognitive Engineering in the Aviation Domain. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005.
SKINNER, B. F. The origins of cognitive thought. American Psychologist, v. 44, p. 13-18, 1989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.1.13
SPERBER, D.; HIRSCHFELD, L. Culture, cognition, and evolution. In: WILSON, R. A. KEIL, F. C. (Eds.). The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2001.
VICENTE, K. J. Commentary on evolutionary perspective on the growth of cognitive engineering: the Riso genotype. Ergonomics, v. 41, n. 2, p. 156-159, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/001401398187189
VICENTE, K. J. Cognitive work analysis: Towards safe, productive and healthy computer-based work. Mahwah: Erlbaum, 1999.
VICENTE, K. J.; RASMUSSEN, J. Ecological interface design: Theoretical Foundations. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, v. 22, n. 4, p. 589-606, 1992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/21.156574
VIDAL, M. C.; CARVALHO, P. V. R. Ergonomia Cognitiva: raciocínio e decisão no trabalho. Rio de Janeiro: EVC Editora, 2008.
VYGOTSKY, L. Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1978.
WAGEMANS, J.; VERSTRATEN, F. A. J.; HE, S. Beyond the decade of the brain: towards a functional neuroanatomy of the mind. Acta Psychologica, v. 107, p. 1-7, 2001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00040-3
WICKENS, C. D. Engineering psychology and human performance. 2nd. New York: Harper Collins, 1992.
WICKENS, C. D. Situation Awareness: review of Mica Endsley's 1995 article on situation awareness theory and measurement. Human Factors, v. 50, p. 397-403, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872008X288420
WICKENS, C.; HOLLAND, J. G. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 2000.
WIENER, E. L. Cockpit automation. In: WIENER, E. L.; NAGEL, D. C. (Eds.). Human factors in aviation. San Diego: Academic, 1988. p. 433-461.
WIENER, E. L.; CURRY, R. E. Flight deck automation: Promises and problems. Ergonomics, v. 23, p. 995-1011, 1980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140138008924809
WOODS, D. D. Process-Tracing Methods for the Study of Cognition Outside of the Experimental Psychology Laboratory. In: KLEIN, G. A. et al. (Eds.). Decision Making in Action: Models and Methods. Mahwah: Ablex Publishing Corporation, 1993.
WOODS, D. D. Discovering how distributed cognitive systems works. In: HOLLNAGEL, E. (Ed.). Handbook of Cognitive Task Design. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2003. p. 37-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781410607775.ch3
WOODS, D. D. Creating foresight: lessons for enhancing resilience from Columbia. In: STARBUCK, W.H.; FARJOUN, M. (Eds.) Organization at the limit: lessons from the Columbia Disaster. Malden: Blackwell Publishing, 2005. p.289-308.
WOODS, D. D.; CHRISTOFFERSEN, K. Balancing practice-centered research and design. In: McNEESE, M. D.; VIDULICH, M. A. (Eds.). State of the art report (SOAR): Cognitive systems engineering in military aviation environments: Avoiding cogminutia fragmentosa. Wright-Patterson AFB: Human Systems Information Analysis Center, 2002. p. 121-134.
WOODS, D. D.; DEKKER, S. W. A. Anticipating the Effects of Technological Change: A New Era of Dynamics for Human Factors. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, v. 1, n. 3, p. 272-282, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14639220110037452
WOODS, D. D. et al. Behind human error. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing Co, 2010.
WOODS, D. D.; HOLLNAGEL, E. Joint cognitive systems: patterns in cognitive systems engineering. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781420005684
WOODS, D. D.; ROTH, E. M. Cognitive engineering: Human problem solving with tools. Human Factors, v. 30, p. 415-430, 1988.
WOODS, D. D. et al. Studying cognitive work in context: Facilitating insight at the intersection of people, technology and work. Cogn. Syst. Eng. Lab. Inst. Ergon.; Ohio State University, Columbus, 2002. Disponível em:
YERKES, R. M.; DODSON, J. D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, v. 18, p. 459-482, 1908. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.920180503